The utility of distributed practice in curriculum design and effective learning strategies

Written by: Lewis A Baker
6 min read
Lewis A Baker, Faculty of Engineering and Physical Science, University of Surrey, UK Distributed practice, a learning strategy that can inform curriculum design, deliberately spaces out opportunities for memory storage and retrieval of taught information to develop deep, robust and long-term learning for students (Dunlosky et al., 2013). Massed practice (better known to students as ‘cramming’), in contrast, involves material being studied for similar periods but without (or with little) spacing between such memory-storage-retrieval opportunities. In such cases, knowledge and skill competency are often forgotten. This translates clearly into assessment, where those who have learned in a ‘distributed curriculum’ compared to a ‘massed curriculum’ can show significant learning gains (Hattie, 2008) from the longer-lasting and deeper learning offered by spacing learning opportunities out, leading to what is referred to as the ‘distributed practice’, ‘spacing’ or ‘lag

Join us or sign in now to view the rest of this page

You're viewing this site as a guest, which only allows you to view a limited amount of content.

To view this page and get access to all our resources, join the Chartered College of Teaching (it's free for trainee teachers and half price for ECTs) or log in if you're already a member.

    • Agarwal PK (2019) Retrieval practice & Bloom’s taxonomy: Do students need fact knowledge before higher order learning? Journal of Educational Psychology 111(2): 189.
    • Baker LA (2020) Releasing students from the cognitive straitjacket of visual-auditory kinaesthetic learning styles. Impact 3(10): 57–60.
    • Benjamin AS and Tullis J (2010) What makes distributed practice effective? Cognitive Psychology 61(3): 228–247.
    • Cowan N (2008) What are the differences between long-term, short-term, and working memory? In: Sossin WS, Lacaille J-C, Castellucci V et al. (eds) Essence of Memory. Elsevier, pp. 323–338.
    • Cowan N (2010) The magical mystery four: How is working memory capacity limited, and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science 19(1): 51–57.
    • Dirkx KJH, Camp G, Kester L et al. (2019) Do secondary school students make use of effective study strategies when they study on their own? Applied Cognitive Psychology 33(5): 952–957.
    • Dunlosky J, Rawson KA, Marsh EJ et al. (2013) Improving students’ learning with effective learning techniques: Promising directions from cognitive and educational psychology. Psychological Science in the Public Interest 14(1): 4–58.
    • Ghosh VE and Gilboa A (2014) What is a memory schema? A historical perspective on current neuroscience literature. Neuropsychologia 53: 104–114.
    • Ginns P (2005) Meta-analysis of the modality effect. Learning and Instruction 15(4): 313–331.
    • Hattie J (2008) Visible Learning: A Synthesis of Over 800 Meta‐Analyses Relating to Achievement. Abingdon: Routledge.
    • Hendrick C and Macpherson R (2017) What Does This Look Like in the Classroom? Bridging the Gap Between Research and Practice. Woodbridge: John Catt Educational Limited.
    • Herrlinger S, Höffler TN, Opfermann M et al. (2017) When Do pictures help learning from expository text? Multimedia and modality effects in primary schools. Research in Science Education 47(3): 685–704.
    • Kirschner PA (2017) Stop propagating the learning styles myth. Computers & Education 106: 166–171.
    • Kirschner PA, Sweller J and Clark RE (2006) Why Minimal guidance during instruction does not work: An Analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist 41(2): 75–86.
    • Latimier A, Peyre H and Ramus F (2020) A meta-analytic review of the benefit of spacing out retrieval practice episodes on retention. Educational Psychology Review 33: 959–987.
    • Nazari KB and Ebersbach M (2019) Distributed practice in mathematics: Recommendable especially for students on a medium performance level? Trends in Neuroscience and Education 17: 100122.
    • Rawson KA, Thomas RC and Jacoby LL (2015) The power of examples: Illustrative examples enhance conceptual learning of declarative concepts. Educational Psychology Review 27(3): 483–504.
    • Roediger HL, Putnam AL and Smith M (2011) Ten Benefits of testing and their applications to educational practice. In Mestre JP and Ross BH (eds) Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Academic Press, pp. 1–36.
    • Rohrer D (2012) Interleaving helps students distinguish among similar concepts. Educational Psychology Review 24(3): 355–367.
    • Weinstein Y, Smith M and Caviglioli O (nd) Six Strategies for effective learning. Available at: www.learningscientists.org/posters (accessed 16 November 2021).
    4 1 vote
    Please Rate this content
    Subscribe
    Notify of
    0 Comments
    Oldest
    Newest Most Voted
    Inline Feedbacks
    View all comments

    From this issue

    Impact Articles on the same themes