How do primary school children progress in understanding the composition of matter?

Written By: Author(s): Maria Tsapali
7 min read
How do primary school children progress in understanding the composition of matter?
Introduction A fundamental target of chemistry education is for students to develop an understanding of the concept of matter and its particulate nature. Yet a number of studies have shown that students find it difficult to understand the concept and to apply it consistently when explaining chemical phenomena (e.g. Nakhleh and Samarapungavan, 1999; Talanquer, 2009). This study aims to explore how young children develop their understanding of the particulate nature of matter. This is an important question, as few studies concern primary school children (Krnel, Watson and Glažar, 2005; Liu and Lesniak, 2005) and the research literature so far offers mixed answers as to the age at which children can understand the particle model (e.g. Fensham, 1994; Harrison and Treagust, 2002; Novak and Musonda, 1991; Papageorgiou and Johnson, 2005). Students’ understanding of scientific concepts Students’ understanding of scientific concepts has been extensively studied in science education r

Join us or sign in now to view the rest of this page

You're viewing this site as a guest, which only allows you to view a limited amount of content.

To view this page and get access to all our resources, join the Chartered College of Teaching (it's free for trainee teachers and half price for ECTs) or log in if you're already a member.

This article was published in July 2019 and reflects the terminology and understanding of research and evidence in use at the time. Some terms and conclusions may no longer align with current standards. We encourage readers to approach the content with an understanding of this context.

References
  • Barlett FC (1932) Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Braun V and Clarke V (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology 3(2): 77–101.
  • Fensham PP (1994) Progression in school science curriculum: A rational prospect or a chimera? Research in Science Education 24(1): 76–82.
  • Hadenfeldt JC, Liu X and Neumann K (2014) Framing students’ progression in understanding matter: a review of previous research. Studies in Science Education 50(2): 181–208.
  • Harrison AG and Treagust DF (2002) The Particulate Nature of Matter: Challenges in Understanding the Submicroscopic World. In Gilbert JK, Jong OD, Justi R, Treagust DF and Driel JHV (eds.) Chemical Education: Towards Research-based Practice Netherlands: Springer, pp. 189–212.
  • Krnel D, Watson R and Glažar SA (2005) The development of the concept of “matter”: A cross‐age study of how children describe materials. International Journal of Science Education 27(3): 367–383.
  • Krnel D, Watson R and Glažar SA (1998) Survey of research related to the development of the concept of 'matter’. International Journal of Science Education 20(3): 257–289.
  • Liu X and Lesniak KM (2005) Students’ progression of understanding the matter concept from elementary to high school. Science Education 89(3): 433–450.
  • Nakhleh MB and Samarapungavan A (1999) Elementary school children’s beliefs about matter. Journal of Research in Science Teaching 36(7): 777–805. Nickerson RS (1985) Understanding Understanding. American Journal of Education 93: 201–239.
  • Novak JD and Musonda D (1991) A Twelve-Year Longitudinal Study of Science Concept Learning. American Educational Research Journal 28(1): 117–153.
  • Papageorgiou G and Johnson P (2005. Do Particle Ideas Help or Hinder Pupils’ Understanding of Phenomena? International Journal of Science Education 27(11): 1299–1317.
  • Quinn C, Ellefson MR and Taber KS (2013) Using property induction to evaluate understanding of dissolving. In Knauff, M., Pauen, M., Sebanz N. and Wachsmuth, I. (eds.) Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society Austin: Cognitive Science Society, pp. 1181-1186.
  • Smith EL (1991) A conceptual change model of learning science. In Glynn, SM Yeany RH andBritton BK (eds.) The Psychology of Learning Science. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 43–63.
  • Stavy R and Stachel D (1985) Children’s ideas about 'solid' and 'liquid.' European Journal of Science Education 7(4): 407–421.
  • Taber KS (2013) Modelling learners and learning in science education: Developing Representations of Concepts, Conceptual Structure and Conceptual Change to Inform Teaching and Research. Dordrecht: Springer.
  • Talanquer V (2009) On Cognitive Constraints and Learning Progressions: The case of “structure of matter.” International Journal of Science Education 31(15): 2123–2136.
  • Tsapali M, Quinn C, Ellefson MR et al. (2018) Progress in primary school children’s understandings of materials and their composition: A cross-age qualitative study. In: Finlayson O, McLoughlin E, Erduran S et al. (eds) Electronic Proceedings of the ESERA 2017 Conference. Research, Practice and Collaboration in Science Education. Dublin, Ireland: Dublin City University, pp. 2167–2180.
0 0 votes
Please Rate this content
Subscribe
Notify of
0 Comments
Oldest
Newest Most Voted
Inline Feedbacks
View all comments

Other content you may be interested in