Impact Journal Logo

Spaced learning: The final frontier in revision

Written by: Stephen Lockyer
5 min read

This is a graphic showing three different learning schedules. The first schedule is titled "Contracting learning schedule". This shows icons of students studying five days and then one day apart. An attached box says: "Students using the contraction learning schedule performed better when tested after: 1 day or 7 days". The second schedule is titled "Equal learning schedule" and shows students studying for three days, followed by three days. The third schedule is titled "Expanding learning schedule" and shows students studying for one day and then five days. Both schedules lead to a box labelled: "Whereas students using equal or expanding learning schedules performed better when tested after a more significant gap: 35 days".

Stephen Lockyer delves into the world of cognitive science to explore what is known about strategies for improving memory retention.

I was disappointed as a child that there wasn’t a GCSE in revision timetabling, as I was that pupil who spent a week making elaborate multi-coloured revision schedules, only to then ignore them and cram at the last minute. They, unlike my actual results, were a thing of beauty.

The strange thing is, the cognitive science behind optimal revision times and patterns supports my system. There are three basic models of revision (almost all research in this case operates on the basis that there is a test at the end), and in cognitive research, revision is actually termed relearning, which is a distinct but important difference. A lot of what we advocate as revision is exactly that — looking again — whereas relearning puts a subtle emphasis on the fact that learning takes both time and depth, not simply repetition.

The model of relearning that I inadvertently used during my GCSEs (and which I guess is quite common) is known as a ‘contracting learning schedule’. The time between initial learning and relearning is reduced each time, so that learning episodes occur closer together. This has very similar strains to the type of cramming that happens across the country, just before Key Stage 2 SATs, yet feels very much like jelly-flinging at times — hoping enough of it sticks, even for a few weeks. Cramming has itself built a reputation, whether fairly or not, as an intensive way of studying without any long-term benefits.

The two other models of relearning also base themselves on time periods: an equal learning schedule is one with regular relearning sessions (such as learning a speech during a weekly coaching session, for example), while the expanding learning schedule is one where the space between learning episodes increases each time.

So which model was right? Which did I want to use as a primary teacher? I have a pedagogical desire that I should help the children relearn for life, rather than for tests, and over the years I have designed a 10:10:10 measure for learning (how can I get the children to recall this in 10 minutes, 10 months and 10 years?). So what should my schedule be for revisiting material, if I want to relearn it for life? And were my intentions too ambitious?

One seminal paper for me came from a book recommended at the #PrimaryRocks conference, called How We Learn by Benedict Carey (2015). In it, the author discussed one paper that found that there were actually optimum times for revisiting material in order to remember it, and this led me to the ‘Contracting, equal and expanding learning schedules’ paper by Küpper-Tetzel, Kapler and Wiseheart (2014), which explored the three models mentioned above.

Like many research papers, it told me that empirical data painted a complex picture in relation to which method was best (thanks a lot, empirical data), and in fact the best method depended on how long after the learning sessions the students were tested. The research pitted the three schedules against each other, with students completing three learning episodes on either a contracting, equal or expanding schedule, then being tested either immediately, after one day, after seven days, or after 35 days. It found that contracting schedules — where the gaps between learning episodes reduced — led to improved performance for students in tests carried out after one day and after seven days; however, equal and expanding learning schedules led to improved performance in tests carried out after 35 days. Put simply, contracting schedules improved short-term retention, but equal or expanding schedules improved longer-term retention — an important point given my aim of relearning for life.

I took this back to my classroom via a very simple spreadsheet. I simply put what I was teaching in a cell, and it reminded me to cover this again (to help the children relearn it) every week. As more subjects are added, previous items are reduced to every fortnight and I simply look at it when planning to build in a starter or small activity to refresh children’s learning.

I’ve found that this drip-feed of reminders is an excellent way of nudging the children’s memories of the lesson. Robert Bjork, who is the expert in this area, recognises that the best point to relearn is at the point of almost forgetting (Storm, Bjork and Bjork, 2008). How we identify this point, however, is currently a very complex matter, so for now I’m sticking with the equal and expanding learning schedule, with all its benefits.

10:10:10 learning

Over the years, I’ve come to realise that we need to learn facts and skills to three specific levels: for the lesson itself, for the inevitable test at the end, and so that this new information can be accessed in ten years’ time.

To that end, I have tried hard to condense learning down into one element (one activity sheet, one slide, one piece of targeted work), using mnemonics for greater recall (see Coffman, 2008) and, where possible, enriched experiences to cement for the long-term retention. If you think back to how you remember something, either in everyday life or in a pub quiz, it can highlight a rich context that forms the basis of that memory. I challenge myself daily to do the same for my own pupils.

 

References

Carey B (2015) How We Learn: Throw Out the Rule Book and Unlock your Brain’s Potential. London: Pan Macmillan.
Coffman JL, Ornstein PA, McCall LE and Curran PJ (2008) Linking teachers’ memory-relevant language and the development of children’s memory skills. Developmental Psychology 44(6): 1640–1654.
Küpper-Tetzel CE, Kapler IV and Wiseheart M (2014) Contracting, equal, and expanding learning schedules: The optimal distribution of learning sessions depends on retention interval. Memory and Cognition 42(5): 729–741.
Storm BC, Bjork EL and Bjork R (2008) Accelerated relearning after retrieval-induced forgetting: The benefit of being forgotten. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 34: 230–236.

Further Reading

For further discussion of the research behind these ideas you might like to dip into these two research papers: Bahrick HP and Hall LK (2005) The importance of retrieval failures to long-term retention: A metacognitive explanation of the spacing effect. Journal of Memory and Language 52: 566–577.
Rawson KA, Dunlosky J and Sciartelli SM (2013) The power of successive relearning: Improving performance on course exams and long-term retention. Educational Psychology Review 25: 523–548.

      0 0 votes
      Please Rate this content
      Subscribe
      Notify of
      0 Comments
      Inline Feedbacks
      View all comments

      From this issue

      Impact Articles on the same themes