Teaching reasoning in Year 5 maths reversed the achievement gap between boys and girls

Written By: Isaac Howarth
8 min read
A three-month project shows how quickly gains can be made

This study is based on work undertaken as part of the Masters in Education degree by Isaac Howarth at Edge Hill University in 2015.

Why teach reasoning?

Over the course of three months, I worked with a group of 19 children in Year 5 with low prior attainment to see what effects there would be after a term of direct and regular instruction in reasoning strategies. Pupils were explicitly taught reasoning skills and made aware of reasoning processes during their daily maths lessons.

Research collated by the Education Endowment Foundation indicates that teaching metacognitive strategies can have a major impact on the rate at which children learn (Education Endowment Foundation, 2017). Research in the underlying theory indicated that metacognition can be learned by hearing and taking part in discussions where concepts and logical rules are discussed, hence choosing to teach reasoning skills. Taking time to teach these thinking skills would help children make better progress by concentrating on the aims of the National Curriculum: to develop reasoning, fluency and problem solving (Department for Education, 2014).

There has been a perception that teaching children to reason in maths lessons is a way to challenge more-able children – that has a hidden implication that is it not always necessary for all children. However, there is a relationship between children’s mathematical reasoning and their achievement in maths at the end of Key Stage 2 (Nunes et al., 2009). This makes teaching reasoning a vital component of a quality maths education for all children.

Initial gaps in achievement

Using the school’s regular assessment cycle, the children completed GL Assessment Progress Test Maths 9 in September of Year 5 (GL Assessment, 2015).

An initial analysis of the PTM 9 baseline data indicated that a gap was opening between boys and girls, and that girls were over-represented at the lower end of the achievement spectrum. On average, English as an Additional Language (EAL) children were achieving well compared with their peers, however there was also a cluster of EAL children at the lower end of the achievement spectrum.

The baseline results indicated that there was a noticeable gap in the achievement of girls compared with boys. It was suspected that some girls had started to turn away from maths because they felt maths was ‘not for me’ or may have lacked confidence to speak up in a maths lesson.  There is research indicating that certain conceptions of what it means to ‘do maths’ can affect a child’s eagerness to learn it. This may mean a child may be reluctant to contribute ideas or may have a fear of ‘getting it wrong’ because they think correct answers are valued in maths – even though the conjecturing, inquiring and reasoning are also important parts (Department for Children, Schools and Families, 2009). For this reason, the role of the teacher’s dispositions is considered in the conclusion of this study.

Changes to teaching

From October to December, the children were gradually introduced to low-threshold, high-ceiling questions to make reasoning accessible to all. These questions were designed to be open-ended so they are highly accessible to children across the range of prior achievement, but also permit more complex and intricate responses to offer challenge (McClure, Woodham and Bothwick, 2011).

Reasoning and pattern recognition were also included as an integrated, regular part of the lesson, rather than being saved for the end as a ‘challenge’ or reserved for a plenary. Children were also expected to continue reasoning about familiar topics during warm-up questions to ensure that procedural fluency was still underpinned by conceptual understanding.

As a starter activity, each lesson began with five questions aimed at developing fluency and understanding, with the fifth being a low-threshold, high-ceiling reasoning question designed to challenge conceptions about mathematical properties. Initially the questions were ‘What’s the same/what’s different?’ style or ‘Which is the odd one out?’

At first, the responses were simpler, such as ‘the circle is the odd one out because it does not have straight sides’. That response opened an opportunity to discuss ‘What is a polygon?’ to elevate the vocabulary. Some children offered slightly more perceptive answers, such as, ‘the square is the odd one out because the other shapes do not have any right angles’.

When this same question was asked three months later, the responses were more sophisticated, with some children recognising that the three polygons all needed base and height measurements to find their area. This was following a lengthy sequence of lessons on what area means, and how it is constructed and represented. However, because of their enhanced reasoning skills, the children were able to handle more complex concepts because they knew how to look for detail in an idea and categorise properties.

Role of teacher as model mathematical thinker

Thinking aloud as I gave more sophisticated responses was a powerful way to model being a mathematical thinker. As children heard me think aloud more, they began to adopt the habit, which was beneficial for when they would evaluate their peers’ ideas. This in turn provided a further opportunity for mathematical reasoning and critical evaluation.

When it came to more restrictive forms of reasoning, the questions were about geometric properties because that was a topic which lent itself to visual representations and the children were already familiar with making generalisations about a collection of properties.

A gradual and growing understanding

Initially, the questions or statements provided for discussion were more straightforward, such as: ‘Always, sometimes, never: A square is a rectangle’. Many children understood why a square is always a rectangle using a more precise definition of rectangle. However, reversing the question, ‘Is a rectangle a square?’, was harder to pick apart. Many children wanted the same answer as before because they believed the process worked the same both ways (a commutativity of sorts), but when I asked for examples of rectangles that were squares, and then examples that were not squares, things changed. They came to realise that a rectangle is only sometimes a square, yet a square is always a rectangle. This re-thinking of how maths works was a powerful tool for getting children to challenge their own thinking and provides examples to help refine their thinking.

For this type of dialogue to take place, the children were taught sentence structures that allowed them to debate mathematical ideas and they were expected to respond to one another’s idea in an evaluative way (Mercer and Howe, 2012). Using simple phrases like ‘What do you think of…idea?’ or giving children ways to respectfully disagree by saying, ‘I disagree/agree with… because…’ made critical thought non-threatening and clarified how conjecturing and testing an idea could lead to its refinement through group talk where everyone knew the rules of discussion.

Over time, the children came to understand that reasoning is what binds mathematics together (Nrich, 2014) and that it needs to be on-going. As children became more aware of what constitutes mathematical thinking and began to apply that rigour to their own thinking, they secured a better understanding of concepts within mathematics and how they work. With a more secure understanding of how mathematics works they started to make more sense of how mathematics can be used to represent and solve real-life problems.

Results and discussion

Amongst the results, it was noticed that EAL children ended up achieving higher scores than non-EAL children and also making a greater gain in reasoning. The same was true for girls, who reversed the gap with boys in both achievement and gains. All children made a substantial amount of progress to reach levels of achievement in line with national averages. There is a positive indication that explicitly teaching mathematical reasoning can also raise the achievement of other groups, as it did with EAL children and girls. Some suggested reasons for the improvements made are discussed below.

This figure is titled "Improvement in mathematical reasoning" and shows a bar graph with red and green bars and a black line for the average result. The horizontal axis has the following groups listed: "All children", "EAL", "Non-EAL", "Girls", and "Boys". The vertical axis ranges from 0 to 70. The red bars are labelled "Reasoning baseline", with the highest value recorded in the "Boys" group and the lowest value recorded in the "Girls" group. The green bars are labelled "Reasoning final", with the highest value recorded in the "EAL" group and the lowest value recorded in the "Non-EAL" group. The black line is labelled "National % correct" and stands at around 55.

The progressive difficulty in the questions posed provided a gradual increase in the level of challenge, and the modelling of mathematical thought allowed children access to the mind of a mathematical thinker. Over time, they internalised mathematical thought structures and would use them to evaluate their classmates’ ideas. This meant direct teaching of language structures for reasoning was necessary, which likely explains the large improvement in EAL children’s performance, since the academic language of mathematical reasoning is unlikely to be acquired outside of a formal school setting. The language of reasoning is highly specific and highly structured, enabling children to learn it quickly.

The gap closing between girls and boys may have been due to a shift in attitude towards what it means to ‘do maths’. The focus shifted towards more reasoning, re-thinking and evaluating. In lessons, collaboration and discussion were valued because they were explicitly planned for, in turn making more time for deeper thought (Mercer and Howe, 2012). This was a big shift in attitude for some boys, who can often be taught that ‘being good’ at maths is about competing for correct answers.

Likewise, more emphasis on re-working a response meant there was less fear of being perceived as slower, ‘wrong’ or ‘losing’ (Department for Children, Schools and Families, 2009). Reducing the competitive element in lessons may have been advantageous for both girls and boys who had learned to be fearful of maths (Department for Children, Schools and Families, 2009), but appears to have had more of an impact on girls.

The short-term nature of this project meant that it remains to be seen if the gains are sustained in the long run; however, it also shows how quickly the gains can be made.

Conclusions

This case study has shown that directly teaching mathematical reasoning by incorporating low-threshold, high ceiling questions into regular curricular lessons and providing scaffolds to develop critical discussion adds much value to children’s learning in mathematics. This study demonstrates that it is possible to teach reasoning alongside fluency, rather than viewing reasoning as an additional challenge. By including reasoning even with basic facts and properties, children come to a richer and better connected understanding of those core facts and properties.

The short-term nature of this project meant that it remains to be seen if the gains are sustained in the long run; however, it also shows how quickly the gains can be made. This project has also demonstrated the influence a teacher has over the classroom climate and children’s attitudes, which is also influenced by how they construct teaching sequences and what they value as a ‘good’ mathematical response and what they espouse as the qualities of a ‘good’ mathematician.

References
0 0 votes
Please Rate this content
Subscribe
Notify of
0 Comments
Oldest
Newest Most Voted
Inline Feedbacks
View all comments

Other content you may be interested in